Turn off MathJax
Article Contents
Lan Zhao, Sheng-Nan Ji, Xiao-Bing Du, Jia-Hui Liu, Bo-Lun Zhang, Pei-Hua Li, Yi-Jun Yang, Bao-Guo Li, Yan-Qing Guo, Xiao-Guang Qi. Dynamic foraging strategy adaptation to heterogeneous environments contributes to social aggregation in snub-nosed monkeys. Zoological Research, 2024, 45(1): 39-54. doi: 10.24272/j.issn.2095-8137.2023.047
Citation: Lan Zhao, Sheng-Nan Ji, Xiao-Bing Du, Jia-Hui Liu, Bo-Lun Zhang, Pei-Hua Li, Yi-Jun Yang, Bao-Guo Li, Yan-Qing Guo, Xiao-Guang Qi. Dynamic foraging strategy adaptation to heterogeneous environments contributes to social aggregation in snub-nosed monkeys. Zoological Research, 2024, 45(1): 39-54. doi: 10.24272/j.issn.2095-8137.2023.047

Dynamic foraging strategy adaptation to heterogeneous environments contributes to social aggregation in snub-nosed monkeys

doi: 10.24272/j.issn.2095-8137.2023.047
Permission for field survey in Mts. Qinling was granted by the Shaanxi Forestry Bureau.
Supplementary data to this article can be found online.
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
X.G.Q. conceived and designed the study. L.Z. and S.N.J. compiled the dataset. L.Z. analyzed the data. X.B.D, J.H.L, B.L.Z., and P.H.L. performed experiments. L.Z., S.N.J., J.H.L., Y.J.Y., B.G.L., Y.Q.G., and X.G.Q. wrote and revised the manuscript. All authors added materials, helped to revise the manuscript, and approved the final manuscript.
#Authors contributed equally to this work
Funds:  This research was supported by the Biodiversity Survey and Assessment Project of the Ministry of Ecology and Environment, China (2019HJ2096001006), National Natural Science Foundation of China (32001099), and China Postdoctoral Science Foundation (2020M683539)
More Information
  • Corresponding author: E-mail: qixg@nwu.edu.cn
  • Received Date: 2023-05-04
  • Accepted Date: 2023-06-21
  • Published Online: 2023-09-09
  • The dynamics of animal social structures are heavily influenced by environmental patterns of competition and cooperation. In folivorous colobine primates, prevailing theories suggest that larger group sizes should be favored in rainforests with a year-round abundance of food, thereby reducing feeding competition. Yet, paradoxically, larger groups are frequently found in high-altitude or high-latitude montane ecosystems characterized by a seasonal scarcity of leaves. This contradiction is posited to arise from cooperative benefits in heterogeneous environments. To investigate this hypothesis, we carried out a six-year field study on two neighboring groups of golden snub-nosed monkey (Rhinopithecus roxellana), a species representing the northernmost distribution of colobine primates. Results showed that the groups adjusted their movement and habitat selection in response to fluctuating climates and spatiotemporal variability of resources, indicative of a dynamic foraging strategy. Notably, during the cold, resource-scarce conditions in winter, the large group occupied food-rich habitats but did not exhibit significantly longer daily travel distances than the smaller neighboring group. Subsequently, we compiled an eco-behavioral dataset of 52 colobine species to explore their evolutionary trajectories. Analysis of this dataset suggested that the increase in group size may have evolved via home range expansion in response to the cold and heterogeneous climates found at higher altitudes or latitudes. Hence, we developed a multi-benefits framework to interpret the formation of larger groups by integrating environmental heterogeneity. In cold and diverse environments, even smaller groups require larger home ranges to meet their dynamic survival needs. The spatiotemporal distribution of high-quality resources within these expanded home ranges facilitates more frequent interactions between groups, thereby encouraging social aggregation into larger groups. This process enhances the benefits of collaborative actions and reproductive opportunities, while simultaneously optimizing travel costs through a dynamic foraging strategy.
  • Permission for field survey in Mts. Qinling was granted by the Shaanxi Forestry Bureau.
    Supplementary data to this article can be found online.
    The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
    X.G.Q. conceived and designed the study. L.Z. and S.N.J. compiled the dataset. L.Z. analyzed the data. X.B.D, J.H.L, B.L.Z., and P.H.L. performed experiments. L.Z., S.N.J., J.H.L., Y.J.Y., B.G.L., Y.Q.G., and X.G.Q. wrote and revised the manuscript. All authors added materials, helped to revise the manuscript, and approved the final manuscript.
    #Authors contributed equally to this work
  • loading
  • [1]
    Adams FV, Arseneau-Robar TJM, Bonnell TR, et al. 2021. Temporal patterns in the social network of core units in Rwenzori Angolan colobus monkeys: Effects of food availability and interunit dispersal. Ecology and Evolution, 11(7): 3251−3263. doi: 10.1002/ece3.7274
    [2]
    Arseneau-Robar TJM, Changasi AH, Turner E, et al. 2021. Diet and activity budget in Colobus angolensis ruwenzorii at nabugabo, uganda: are they energy maximizers?. Folia Primatologica, 92(1): 35−48.
    [3]
    Beyer DE JR, Haufler JB. 1994. Diurnal versus 24-hour sampling of habitat use. The Journal of Wildlife Management, 58(1): 178−180. doi: 10.2307/3809566
    [4]
    Bjørneraas K, Van Moorter B, Rolandsen CM, et al. 2010. Screening global positioning system location data for errors using animal movement characteristics. The Journal of Wildlife Management, 74(6): 1361−1366. doi: 10.1111/j.1937-2817.2010.tb01258.x
    [5]
    Chapman CA, Chapman LJ. 2000. Determinants of group size in primates: the importance of travel costs. In: Boinski S, Garber PA. On the Move: How and Why Animals Travel in Groups. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 24–41.
    [6]
    Chapman CA, Teichroeb JA. 2012. What influences the size of groups in which primates choose to live?. Nature Education Knowledge, 3(10): 9.
    [7]
    Clutton-Brock T. 2021. Social evolution in mammals. Science, 373(6561): eabc9699. doi: 10.1126/science.abc9699
    [8]
    Clutton-Brock T, Janson C. 2012. Primate socioecology at the crossroads: Past, present, and future. Evolutionary Anthropology:Issues, News, and Reviews, 21(4): 136−150. doi: 10.1002/evan.21316
    [9]
    Davies AB, Ancrenaz M, Oram F, et al. 2017. Canopy structure drives orangutan habitat selection in disturbed Bornean forests. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 114(31): 8307−8312.
    [10]
    Fan PL, Li YM, Stanford CB, et al. 2019. Home range variation of two different-sized groups of golden snub-nosed monkeys (Rhinopithecus roxellana) in Shennongjia, China: implications for feeding competition. Zoological Research, 40(2): 121−128. doi: 10.24272/j.issn.2095-8137.2018.044
    [11]
    Fang G, Chen J, Pan RL, et al. 2018a. Female choice impacts residential male takeover in golden snub-nosed monkeys (Rhinopithecus roxellana). Zoological Research, 39(4): 266−271. doi: 10.24272/j.issn.2095-8137.2018.035
    [12]
    Fang G, Li M, Liu XJ, et al. 2018b. Preliminary report on Sichuan golden snub-nosed monkeys (Rhinopithecus roxellana roxellana) at Laohegou Nature Reserve, Sichuan, China. Scientific Reports, 8(1): 16183. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-34311-z
    [13]
    Ganas J, Robbins MM. 2005. Ranging behavior of the mountain gorillas (Gorilla beringei beringei) in Bwindi Impenetrable National Park, Uganda: a test of the ecological constraints model. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 58(3): 277−288. doi: 10.1007/s00265-005-0920-z
    [14]
    Garcia C, Bouret S, Druelle F, et al. 2021. Balancing costs and benefits in primates: ecological and palaeoanthropological views. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences, 376(1819): 20190667. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2019.0667
    [15]
    Grove M. 2012. Space, time, and group size: a model of constraints on primate social foraging. Animal Behaviour, 83(2): 411−419. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.11.011
    [16]
    Grow NB, Gursky-Doyen S, Krzton A. 2014. High Altitude Primates. New York: Springer.
    [17]
    Grueter CC. 2022. Causes and consequences of the formation of Multilevel Societies in Colobines. In: Matsuda I, Grueter CC, Teichroeb JA. The Colobines: Natural History, Behaviour and Ecological Diversity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 293–311.
    [18]
    Grueter CC, Li DY, Ren BP, et al. 2013. Overwintering strategy of Yunnan snub-nosed monkeys: adjustments in activity scheduling and foraging patterns. Primates, 54(2): 125−135. doi: 10.1007/s10329-012-0333-3
    [19]
    Grueter CC, Li DY, Van Schaik CP, et al. 2008. Ranging of Rhinopithecus bieti in the samage forest, China. I. Characteristics of range use. International Journal of Primatology, 29(5): 1121−1145. doi: 10.1007/s10764-008-9299-9
    [20]
    Grueter CC, Qi XG, Li BG, et al. 2017. Multilevel societies. Current Biology, 27(18): R984−R986. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.06.063
    [21]
    Grueter CC, Qi XG, Zinner D, et al. 2020. Multilevel organisation of animal sociality. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 35(9): 834−847.
    [22]
    Grueter CC, Van Schaik CP. 2010. Evolutionary determinants of modular societies in colobines. Behavioral Ecology, 21(1): 63−71. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arp149
    [23]
    Hemingway CA, Bynum N. 2005. The influence of seasonality on primate diet and ranging. In: Brockman DK, Van Schaik CP. Seasonality in Primates: Studies of Living and Extinct Human and Non-Human Primates. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 57–104.
    [24]
    Huang PZ, Bian K, Huang ZP, et al. 2021. Human activities and elevational constraints restrict ranging patterns of snub-nosed monkeys in a mountainous refuge. Integrative Zoology, 16(2): 202−213. doi: 10.1111/1749-4877.12490
    [25]
    Huang ZP. 2015. Ecological Mechanism of Fission-Fusion of Golden Snub-Nosed Monkey (Rhinopithecus roxellana) in Qingling Mountains. Ph. D. dissertation, Northwest University, Xi’an.
    [26]
    Isbell LA. 1991. Contest and scramble competition: patterns of female aggression and ranging behavior among primates. Behavioral Ecology, 2(2): 143−155. doi: 10.1093/beheco/2.2.143
    [27]
    Isbell LA, Young TP. 2002. Ecological models of female social relationships in primates: similarities, disparities, and some directions for future clarity. Behaviour, 139(2-3): 177−202.
    [28]
    Janson CH, Goldsmith ML. 1995. Predicting group size in primates: foraging costs and predation risks. Behavioral Ecology, 6(3): 326−336. doi: 10.1093/beheco/6.3.326
    [29]
    Kappeler PM. 2019. A framework for studying social complexity. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 73(1): 13. doi: 10.1007/s00265-018-2601-8
    [30]
    Kappeler PM, Clutton-Brock T, Shultz S, et al. 2019. Social complexity: patterns, processes, and evolution. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 73(1): 5. doi: 10.1007/s00265-018-2613-4
    [31]
    Kraus JB, Strier KB. 2022. Geographic, climatic, and phylogenetic drivers of variation in colobine activity budgets. Primates, 63(6): 647−658. doi: 10.1007/s10329-022-01015-y
    [32]
    Kreft H, Jetz W. 2007. Global patterns and determinants of vascular plant diversity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104(14): 5925−5930.
    [33]
    Li BG, Chen C, Ji WH, et al. 2000. Seasonal home range changes of the Sichuan snub-nosed monkey (Rhinopithecus roxellana) in the Qinling Mountains of China. Folia Primatologica, 71(6): 375−386.
    [34]
    Li BG, Pan RL, Oxnard CE. 2002. Extinction of Snub-Nosed Monkeys in China During the Past 400 Years. International Journal of Primatology, 23(6): 1227−1244. doi: 10.1023/A:1021122819845
    [35]
    Li BG, Ren BP, Gao YF. 1999. A change in the summer home range of Sichuan snub-nosed monkeys in Yuhuangmiao, Qinling mountains. Folia Primatologica, 70(5): 269−273. doi: 10.1159/000021706
    [36]
    Li YL, Wang L, Wu JW, et al. 2020. Bachelor groups in primate multilevel society facilitate gene flow across fragmented habitats. Current Zoology, 66(2): 113−122. doi: 10.1093/cz/zoaa006
    [37]
    Li YM. 2006. Seasonal variation of diet and food availability in a group of Sichuan snub-nosed monkeys in Shennongjia Nature Reserve, China. American Journal of Primatology, 68(3): 217−233. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20220
    [38]
    Markham AC, Gesquiere LR. 2017. Costs and benefits of group living in primates: an energetic perspective. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B:Biological Sciences, 372(1727): 20160239. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0239
    [39]
    Miller A, Judge D, Uwingeneye G, et al. 2020. Diet and use of fallback foods by rwenzori black-and-white colobus (Colobus angolensis ruwenzorii) in rwanda: implications for supergroup formation. International Journal of Primatology, 41(3): 434−457. doi: 10.1007/s10764-020-00143-w
    [40]
    Pyke GH, Pulliam HR, Charnov EL. 1977. Optimal foraging: A selective review of theory and tests. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 52(2): 137−154. doi: 10.1086/409852
    [41]
    Qi XG, Garber PA, Ji WH, et al. 2014. Satellite telemetry and social modeling offer new insights into the origin of primate multilevel societies. Nature Communications, 5(1): 5296. doi: 10.1038/ncomms6296
    [42]
    Qi XG, Grueter CC, Fang G, et al. 2020. Multilevel societies facilitate infanticide avoidance through increased extrapair matings. Animal Behaviour, 161: 127−137. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2019.12.014
    [43]
    Qi XG, Huang K, Fang G, et al. 2017. Male cooperation for breeding opportunities contributes to the evolution of multilevel societies. Proceedings of the Royal Society B:Biological Sciences, 284(1863): 20171480. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2017.1480
    [44]
    Qi XG, Li BG, Garber PA, et al. 2009. Social dynamics of the golden snub-nosed monkey (Rhinopithecus roxellana): female transfer and one-male unit succession. American Journal of Primatology, 71(8): 670−679. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20702
    [45]
    Qi XG, Li BG, Ji WH. 2008. Reproductive parameters of wild female Rhinopithecus roxellana. American Journal of Primatology, 70(4): 311–319.
    [46]
    Qi XG, Wu JW, Zhao L, et al. 2023. Adaptations to a cold climate promoted social evolution in Asian colobine primates. Science, 380(6648): eabl8621. doi: 10.1126/science.abl8621
    [47]
    R Core Team. 2022. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
    [48]
    Ren BP, Li M, Long YC, et al. 2009a. Influence of day length, ambient temperature, and seasonality on daily travel distance in the Yunnan snub-nosed monkey at Jinsichang, Yunnan, China. American Journal of Primatology, 71(3): 233−241. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20641
    [49]
    Ren BP, Li M, Long YC, et al. 2009b. Home range and seasonality of Yunnan snub-nosed monkeys. Integrative Zoology, 4(2): 162−171. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-4877.2009.00153.x
    [50]
    Rowe N, Myers M. 2017. All the World’s Primates. Charlestown: Primate Conservation.
    [51]
    Schreier AL, Swedell L. 2012. The socioecology of network scaling ratios in the multilevel society of hamadryas baboons (Papio hamadryas hamadryas). International Journal of Primatology, 33(5): 1069−1080. doi: 10.1007/s10764-011-9572-1
    [52]
    Shen SF, Emlen ST, Koenig WD, et al. 2017. The ecology of cooperative breeding behaviour. Ecology Letters, 20(6): 708−720. doi: 10.1111/ele.12774
    [53]
    Snaith TV, Chapman CA. 2005. Towards an ecological solution to the folivore paradox: patch depletion as an indicator of within-group scramble competition in red colobus monkeys (Piliocolobus tephrosceles). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 59(2): 185−190. doi: 10.1007/s00265-005-0023-x
    [54]
    Snyder-Mackler N, Beehner JC, Bergman TJ. 2012. Defining higher levels in the multilevel societies of geladas (Theropithecus gelada). International Journal of Primatology, 33(5): 1054−1068. doi: 10.1007/s10764-012-9584-5
    [55]
    Steenbeek R, Van Schaik CP. 2001. Competition and group size in Thomas’s langurs (Presbytis thomasi): the folivore paradox revisited. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 49(2-3): 100−110. doi: 10.1007/s002650000286
    [56]
    Sterck EHM, Roth TS. 2022. Socioecology of asian colobines. In: Matsuda I, Grueter CC, Teichroeb JA. The Colobines: Natural History, Behaviour and Ecological Diversity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 250–270.
    [57]
    Symonds MRE, Blomberg SP. 2014. A primer on phylogenetic generalised least squares. In: Garamszegi LZ. Modern Phylogenetic Comparative Methods and Their Application in Evolutionary Biology: Concepts and Practice. Berlin: Springer, 105–130.
    [58]
    Tan CL, Guo ST, Li BG. 2007. Population structure and ranging patterns of Rhinopithecus roxellana in Zhouzhi national nature reserve, Shaanxi, China. International Journal of Primatology, 28(3): 577−591. doi: 10.1007/s10764-007-9147-3
    [59]
    Tan CL, Li YM, Li BG. 2016. Rhinopithecus roxellana. In: Rowe N and Myers M. All the World's Primates. Charlestown, Rhode Island: Pogonias Press, 625–626.
    [60]
    Teichroeb JA, Sicotte P. 2009. Test of the ecological-constraints model on ursine colobus monkeys (Colobus vellerosus) in Ghana. American Journal of Primatology, 71(1): 49−59. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20617
    [61]
    Tomkiewicz SM, Fuller MR, Kie JG, et al. 2010. Global positioning system and associated technologies in animal behaviour and ecological research. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B:Biological Sciences, 365(1550): 2163−2176. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0090
    [62]
    Van Der Bijl W. 2018. phylopath: Easy phylogenetic path analysis in R. PeerJ, 6: e4718. doi: 10.7717/peerj.4718
    [63]
    Van Moorter B, Rolandsen CM, Basille M, et al. 2016. Movement is the glue connecting home ranges and habitat selection. Journal of Animal Ecology, 85(1): 21−31. doi: 10.1111/1365-2656.12394
    [64]
    Watanabe K. 1981. Variations in group composition and population density of the two sympatric Mentawaian leaf-monkeys. Primates, 22(2): 145−160. doi: 10.1007/BF02382606
    [65]
    Yu Y, He G, Li DY, et al. 2022. Climate change challenge, extinction risk, and successful conservation experiences for a threatened primate species in China: golden snub-nosed monkey (Rhinopithecus roxellana). Zoological Research, 43(6): 940−944. doi: 10.24272/j.issn.2095-8137.2022.198
    [66]
    Zhang KC, Zhou QH, Xu HL, et al. 2021. Diet, food availability, and climatic factors drive ranging behavior in white-headed langurs in the limestone forests of Guangxi, southwest China. Zoological Research, 42(4): 406−411. doi: 10.24272/j.issn.2095-8137.2020.292
  • ZR-2023-047-Supplementary Materials.zip
  • 加载中

Catalog

    通讯作者: 陈斌, bchen63@163.com
    • 1. 

      沈阳化工大学材料科学与工程学院 沈阳 110142

    1. 本站搜索
    2. 百度学术搜索
    3. 万方数据库搜索
    4. CNKI搜索

    Figures(6)

    Article Metrics

    Article views (308) PDF downloads(36) Cited by()
    Proportional views
    Related

    /

    DownLoad:  Full-Size Img  PowerPoint
    Return
    Return